Children develop a veil of fairness

Research output: Contribution to journalJournal articleResearchpeer-review

Standard

Children develop a veil of fairness. / Shaw, Alex; Montinari, Natalia; Piovesan, Marco; Olson, Kristina R.; Gino, Francesca; Norton, Michael I.

In: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Vol. 143, No. 1, 2014, p. 363-375.

Research output: Contribution to journalJournal articleResearchpeer-review

Harvard

Shaw, A, Montinari, N, Piovesan, M, Olson, KR, Gino, F & Norton, MI 2014, 'Children develop a veil of fairness', Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, vol. 143, no. 1, pp. 363-375. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031247

APA

Shaw, A., Montinari, N., Piovesan, M., Olson, K. R., Gino, F., & Norton, M. I. (2014). Children develop a veil of fairness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 363-375. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031247

Vancouver

Shaw A, Montinari N, Piovesan M, Olson KR, Gino F, Norton MI. Children develop a veil of fairness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2014;143(1):363-375. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031247

Author

Shaw, Alex ; Montinari, Natalia ; Piovesan, Marco ; Olson, Kristina R. ; Gino, Francesca ; Norton, Michael I. / Children develop a veil of fairness. In: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2014 ; Vol. 143, No. 1. pp. 363-375.

Bibtex

@article{f30630cc1e7f46459b7802be5108bcd7,
title = "Children develop a veil of fairness",
abstract = "Previous research suggests that children develop an increasing concern with fairness over the course of development. Research with adults suggests that the concern with fairness has at least 2 distinct components: a desire to be fair and a desire to signal to others that they are fair. We explore whether children's developing concern with behaving fairly toward others may in part reflect a developing concern with appearing fair to others. In Experiments 1 and 2, most 6- to 8-year-old children behaved fairly toward others when an experimenter was aware of their choices; fewer children opted to behave fairly, however, when they could be unfair to others yet appear fair to the experimenter. In Experiment 3, we explored the development of this concern with appearing fair by using a wider age range (6- to 11-year-olds) and a different method. In this experiment, children chose how to assign a good or bad prize to themselves and another participant by either unilaterally deciding who would get each prize or using a fair procedure-flipping a coin in private. Older children were much more likely to flip the coin than younger children, yet were just as likely as younger children to assign themselves the good prize by reporting winning the coin flip more than chance would dictate. Overall, the results of these experiments suggest that as children grow older they become increasingly concerned with appearing fair to others, which may explain some of their increased tendency to behave fairly. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved).",
author = "Alex Shaw and Natalia Montinari and Marco Piovesan and Olson, {Kristina R.} and Francesca Gino and Norton, {Michael I.}",
note = "Published online 14 January 2013",
year = "2014",
doi = "10.1037/a0031247",
language = "English",
volume = "143",
pages = "363--375",
journal = "Journal of Experimental Psychology: General",
issn = "0096-3445",
publisher = "American Psychological Association",
number = "1",

}

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Children develop a veil of fairness

AU - Shaw, Alex

AU - Montinari, Natalia

AU - Piovesan, Marco

AU - Olson, Kristina R.

AU - Gino, Francesca

AU - Norton, Michael I.

N1 - Published online 14 January 2013

PY - 2014

Y1 - 2014

N2 - Previous research suggests that children develop an increasing concern with fairness over the course of development. Research with adults suggests that the concern with fairness has at least 2 distinct components: a desire to be fair and a desire to signal to others that they are fair. We explore whether children's developing concern with behaving fairly toward others may in part reflect a developing concern with appearing fair to others. In Experiments 1 and 2, most 6- to 8-year-old children behaved fairly toward others when an experimenter was aware of their choices; fewer children opted to behave fairly, however, when they could be unfair to others yet appear fair to the experimenter. In Experiment 3, we explored the development of this concern with appearing fair by using a wider age range (6- to 11-year-olds) and a different method. In this experiment, children chose how to assign a good or bad prize to themselves and another participant by either unilaterally deciding who would get each prize or using a fair procedure-flipping a coin in private. Older children were much more likely to flip the coin than younger children, yet were just as likely as younger children to assign themselves the good prize by reporting winning the coin flip more than chance would dictate. Overall, the results of these experiments suggest that as children grow older they become increasingly concerned with appearing fair to others, which may explain some of their increased tendency to behave fairly. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved).

AB - Previous research suggests that children develop an increasing concern with fairness over the course of development. Research with adults suggests that the concern with fairness has at least 2 distinct components: a desire to be fair and a desire to signal to others that they are fair. We explore whether children's developing concern with behaving fairly toward others may in part reflect a developing concern with appearing fair to others. In Experiments 1 and 2, most 6- to 8-year-old children behaved fairly toward others when an experimenter was aware of their choices; fewer children opted to behave fairly, however, when they could be unfair to others yet appear fair to the experimenter. In Experiment 3, we explored the development of this concern with appearing fair by using a wider age range (6- to 11-year-olds) and a different method. In this experiment, children chose how to assign a good or bad prize to themselves and another participant by either unilaterally deciding who would get each prize or using a fair procedure-flipping a coin in private. Older children were much more likely to flip the coin than younger children, yet were just as likely as younger children to assign themselves the good prize by reporting winning the coin flip more than chance would dictate. Overall, the results of these experiments suggest that as children grow older they become increasingly concerned with appearing fair to others, which may explain some of their increased tendency to behave fairly. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved).

U2 - 10.1037/a0031247

DO - 10.1037/a0031247

M3 - Journal article

C2 - 23317084

VL - 143

SP - 363

EP - 375

JO - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

JF - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

SN - 0096-3445

IS - 1

ER -

ID: 44185468